### TL;DR This paper explores the fascinating connection betwee...
> ***"The beauty of mathematical formulations lies in abstracting, ...
You can find the Data Sheet with all of the 60 mathematical equatio...
$$1+e^{i\pi}=0$$ Euler's Identity is a special case of Euler's F...
The results show a strong positive correlation between the level of...
Expert mathematicians had an emotional response described as an "ae...
Art and mathematics are sometimes perceived as polar opposites. The...
> ***"Perhaps one of the most awkward, and at the same time challen...
ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE
published: 13 February 2014
doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00068
The experience of mathematical beauty and its neural
correlates
Semir Zeki
1
*
, John Paul Romaya
1
, Dionigi M. T. Benincasa
2
and Michael F. Atiyah
3
1
Wellcome Laboratory of Neurobiology, University College London, London, UK
2
Department of Physics, Imperial College London, London, UK
3
School of Mathematics, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
Edited by:
Josef Parvizi, Stanford University,
USA
Reviewed by:
Miriam Rosenberg-Lee, Stanford
University, USA
Marie Arsalidou, The Hospital for
Sick Children, Canada
*Correspondence:
Semir Zeki, Wellcome Department
of Neurobiology, University College
London, Gower Street, London,
WC1E 6BT, UK
e-mail: s.zeki@ucl.ac.uk
Many have written of the experience of mathematical beauty as being comparable to that
derived from the greatest art. This makes it interesting to learn whether the experience
of beauty derived from such a highly intellectual and abstract source as mathematics
correlates with activity in the same part of the emotional brain as that derived from more
sensory, perceptually based, sources. To determine this, we used functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI) to image the activity in the brains of 15 mathematicians
when they viewed mathematical formulae which they had individually rated as beautiful,
indifferent or ugly. Results showed that the experience of mathematical beauty correlates
parametrically with activity in the same part of the emotional brain, namely field A1 of
the medial orbito-frontal cortex (mOFC), as the experience of beauty derived from other
sources.
Keywords: mathematics, neuroesthetics, fMRI, beauty, mofc
INTRODUCTION
“Mathematics, rightly viewed, possesses not only truth, but
supreme beauty”
Bertrand Russell, Mysticism and Logic (1919).
The beauty of mathematical formulations lies in abstracting, in
simple equations, truths that have universal validity. Many—
among them the mathematicians Bertrand Russell (1919) and
Hermann Weyl (Dyson, 1956; Atiyah, 2002), the physicist Paul
Dirac (1939) and the art critic Clive Bell (1914)—have written of
the importance of beauty in mathematical formulations and have
compared the experience of mathematical beauty to that derived
from the greatest art (Atiyah, 1973). Their descriptions suggest
that the experience of mathematical beauty has much in common
with that derived from other sources, even though mathematical
beauty has a much deeper intellectual source than visual or musi-
cal beauty, which are more “sensible and perceptually based. Past
brain imaging studies exploring the neurobiology of beauty have
shown that the experience of visual (Kawabata and Zeki, 2004),
musical (Blood et al., 1999; Ishizu and Zeki, 2011), and moral
(Tsukiura and Cabeza, 2011) beauty all correlate with activity in a
specific part of the emotional brain, field A1 of the medial orbito-
frontal cortex, which probably includes segments of Brodmann
Areas (BA) 10, 12 and 32 (see Ishizu and Zeki, 2011 for a review).
Our hypothesis in this study was that the experience of beauty
derived from so abstract an intellectual source as mathematics will
correlate with activity in the same part of the emotional brain as
that of beauty derived from other sources.
Plato (1929) thought that “nothing without understanding
would ever be more beauteous than with understanding, mak-
ing mathematical beauty, for him, the highest form of beauty.
The premium thus placed on the faculty of understanding when
experiencing beauty creates both a problem and an opportunity
for studying the neurobiology of beauty. Unlike our previous
studies of the neurobiology of musical or visual beauty, in which
participating subjects were neither experts nor trained in these
domains, in the present study we had, of necessity, to recruit
subjects with a fairly advanced knowledge of mathematics and
a comprehension of the formulae that they viewed and rated.
It is relatively easy to separ ate out the faculty of understanding
from the experience of beauty in mathematics, but much more
difficult to do so for the experience of visual or musical beauty;
hence a study of the neurobiology of mathematical beauty carried
with it the promise of addressing a broader issue with implica-
tions for future studies of the neurobiology of beauty, namely
the extent to which the experience of beauty is bound to that of
“understanding.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Sixteen mathematicians (3 females, age range = 22–32 years, 1
left-handed) at postgraduate or postdoctoral level, all recruited
from colleges in London, took part in the study. All gave
written informed consent and the study was approved by the
Ethics Committee of University College London. All had nor-
mal or corrected to normal vision. One subject was eliminated
from the study after it transpired that he suffered from atten-
tion deficit hyperactivity disorder and had been on medication,
although his exclusion did not affect the overall results. We
also recruited 12 non-mathematicians who completed the ques-
tionnaires described below but were not scanned, for reasons
explained below.
EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURE
To allow a direct comparison between this study and previous
ones in which we explored brain activity that correlates with
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 68
| 1
HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE
Zeki et al. Neural correlates of mathematical beauty
the experience of visual and musical beaut y (Kawabata and Zeki,
2004; Ishizu and Zeki, 2011), we used similar experimental pro-
cedures to these previous studies. About 2–3 weeks before the
scanning experiment, each subject was given 60 mathematical
formulae (Data Sheet 1: EquationsForm.pdf) to study at leisure
and rate on a scale of 5 (ugly) to +5 (beautiful) according
to how beautiful they experienced them to be. Two weeks later,
they participated in a brain scanning experiment, using func-
tional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), during which they
were asked to re-rate the same equations while viewing them
in a Siemens scanner, on an abridged scale of ugly—neutral—
beautiful. The pre-scan ratings were used to balance the sequence
of stimuli for each subject to achieve an even distribution of pre-
ferred and non-preferred equations throughout the experiment.
A few days after scanning, each subject received a questionnaire
(Data Sheet 2: UnderstandingForm.pdf) asking them to (a) report
their level of understanding of each equation on a numerical
scale, from 0 (no understanding) to 3 (profound understanding)
and (b) to report their subjective feelings (including emotional
reaction) when viewing the equations. The data from these ques-
tionnaires (pre-scan beauty ratings, scan-time beauty ratings,
and post-scan understanding ratings) is given in Data Sheet 3:
BehavioralData.xls.
STIMULI
Stimuli consisting of equations were generated using Cogent 2000
(http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/Cogent2000) and displayed by an
Epson EH-TW5910 LCD projector at a resolution of 1600 × 1200
with a refresh rate of 60 Hz. The display was back-projected onto
a translucent screen (290 × 180 mm, 27.2
× 18.1
visual angle),
which was viewed by subjects using an angled mirror.
SCANNING
Subjects viewed the formulae during four functional scanning
sessions, with breaks between sessions which gave them an oppor-
tunity to take a rest if required and us to correct any anoma-
lies, for example to correct rare omissions in rating a stimulus.
Scans were acquired using a 3-T Siemens Magnetom Trio MRI
scanner fitted with a 32-channel head volume coil (Siemens,
Erlangen, Germany). A B0 fieldmap was acquired using a double-
echo FLASH (GRE) sequence (duration 2
14

). An echo-planar
imaging (EPI) sequence was applied for functional scans, measur-
ing BOLD (Blood Oxygen Level Dependent) signals (echo time
TE = 30 ms, TR = 68 ms, volume time = 3.264 s). Each brain
image was acquired in an ascending sequence comprising 48
axial slices, each 2 mm thick, with an interstitial gap of 1 mm
and a voxel resolution of 3 × 3 × 3 mm, covering nearly the
whole brain. After functional scanning had been completed a
T1 MDEFT (modified driven equilibrium fourier transform)
anatomical scan was a cquired in the saggital plane to obtain
high resolution structural images (176 slices per volume, constant
isotropic resolution 1 mm, TE = 2.48 ms, TR = 7.92 ms).
Fifteen equations were displayed during each session
(Figure 1A), so that each of the 60 equations appeared once over
the four sessions. Each session started with a blank gray screen
for 19.5 s, followed by 15 trials, each of 16 s, interspersed with
four blanks, each of 16–17 s, to acquire baseline signal. A plain
gray blank screen was used, of an equivalent overall brightness
to the equation screens. Pre-scan beauty ratings were used to
divide the 60 equations into three groups; 20 “low” rated, 20
“medium rated, and 20 high rated equations. The sequence
of equations viewed by each subject in the scanner was then
organized so that 5 low-, 5 medium- and 5 highly-rated equations
appeared in each session and the pseudo-randomized sequence
was organized so that a low-rated equation was never followed by
another low-rated equation and the same held for medium and
highly rated equations. The session ended with a blank screen of
duration 30 s.
Each trial (Figure 1B) began w ith an equation which was dis-
played for 16 s followed by a blank lasting 1 s. The response screen
appeared for 3 s, during which the subject selected interactively
a scan-time beauty rating (Beautiful, Neutral, or Ugly) for each
equation by pressing keypad buttons. A second blank lasting 1–2 s
ended each trial. Equations were all drawn in the same sized font
in white (CIE 1931 XYZ: 755, 761, 637) and the same gr ay back-
ground was used throughout (CIE 1931 XYZ: 236, 228, 200). The
overall screen brightness varied between 280 and 324 cd m
2
;the
width of equations varied from 4
to 24
visual angle and the
height varied between 1
and 5
.
ANALYSIS
SPM8 (Statistical Parametric Mapping, Friston et al., 2006)was
used to analyze the results, as in our previous studies (Zeki and
Romaya, 2010; Ishizu and Zeki, 2011). At the single-subject level
the understanding rating (0–3) and the scan-time beauty rating
(coded as 1 for “Ugly, 0 for “Neutral, and 1 for “Beautiful”)
for each equation were included as first and second paramet-
ric modulators, respectively, of a boxcar function which modeled
the appearance of each equation [in fact, the beauty and under-
standing ratings correlated but imperfectly (see behavioral data
below)]. There were fewer “Ugly” rated equations than “Neutral”
or “Beautiful” (see behavioral data below). Indeed, in 2 of the
60 functional sessions there was no “Ugly” rating. This imbal-
ance does not bias the estimation of, or inference about, the
effects of beauty—it only reduces the efficiency with which the
effects can be estimated (Friston et al., 2000). Happily, this reduc-
tion was not severe, because we were able to identify significant
effects. As a result of SPM orthogonalization, the beauty rating
parametric modulator can only capture variance that cannot be
explained by the understanding rating, thus allowing us to dis-
tinguish activations that correlate with beauty alone. Contrast
images for each of the 15 subjects for the parametric beauty rat-
ing and for All equations vs. Baseline were taken to a 2nd-level
(random effects) analysis. We used a conjunction-null analysis
(Nichols et al., 2005), to determine whether there was an overlap,
within mOFC, in regions of parametric activation with beauty
and the general de-activation produced by viewing mathemati-
cal formulae. In order to examine the activity of Ugly, Neutral,
and Beautiful stimuli relative to baseline at locations identified in
the parametric beauty analysis we also carried out a categorical
analysis of the beauty rating alone, by coding contrasts for Ugly,
Neutral, and Beautiful stimuli vs. Baseline for each subject at the
first level and taking these to a 2nd level, random effects, analysis
as before.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 68
| 2
Zeki et al. Neural correlates of mathematical beauty
FIGURE 1 | Experimental paradigm and stimulus presentation. There
were four functional scanning sessions, with a break between sessions.
(A) 15 equations were displayed during each session, with each of the 60
equations appearing only once over the four sessions. The pre-scan beauty
ratings were used to sequence the equations for each subject so that 5
low-rated (L), 5 medium-rated (M), and 5 highly-rated equations (H)
appeared in each session and their sequence within a session organized
so that a low-rated equation was never followed by another low-rated
equation (similarly for medium and highly-rated equations). During each
session four blank periods, each varying in duration from 16 to 17 s, were
inserted to collect baseline signal. Each session commenced with a 19.5 s
blank period to allow T1 equilibration effects to subside and ended with a
30 s blank period, giving a tot al duration of about 7 min 20 s per functional
session. (B) Each equation trial began with an equation displayed for 16 s,
followed by a response period during which subjects indicated their
scan-time beauty rating by pressing one of three keypad buttons. This
started with a 1 s blank period (B1) followed by 3 s during which subjects
pressed keypad buttons and their selection (either “Ugly, “Neutral, or
“Beautiful”) was interactively displayed to them. The response was
followed by an inter-trial interval which was randomly varied between 1
and 2 s during which a blank screen appeared (B2). Blank periods displayed
a uniform mid-gray background.
In a similar way, we undertook another parametric analysis
with the scan-time beauty rating as the first parametric mod-
ulator and the understanding rating as the second one. This
time the understanding modulator can only capture variance that
cannot be explained by the beauty rating, thus allowing us to
distinguish activations that are accounted for by understanding
alone. Contrast images for the 15 subjects were taken to the sec-
ond level, as before. To supplement this we also undertook a
categorical analysis of the four understanding categories (0–3) in
order to obtain parameter estimates for the four understanding
categories at locations identified as significant in the parametric
understanding analysis (see Figure 5B).
A categorical analysis differs from the corresponding paramet-
ric analysis in two respects: first, the parametric analysis looks
for a relationship between BOLD signal and differences in the
rated quantity (beauty or understanding) on an individual ses-
sion basis, while a categorical analysis will average the BOLD
signal for each category of the rated quantity over all sessions
and subjects; second, when using two parametric modulators we
can isolate beauty effects from understanding and vice versa by
using orthogonalization, but this is not available in a categori-
cal analysis. When we use orthogonalization of two parametric
regressors to partition the variance in the BOLD signal into a
“beauty only” and an “understanding only” component, there
remains a common portion which cannot be directly attributed
to either component.
For the main contrasts of interest, parametric, and cate-
gorical beauty, we report activation at cluster-level significance
(P
Clust -FWE
< 0.05) with familywise error correction over the
whole br ain volume as reported by SPM8 based on random field
theory (Friston et al., 1994). A statistical threshold of P
unc.
<
0.001 and an extent threshold of 10 voxels was used to define
clusters. There were also some activations which did not reach sig-
nificance (clearly noted) which we nevertheless report since they
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 68
| 3
Zeki et al. Neural correlates of mathematical beauty
suggest areas which may be contributing to the main activation.
Whether they actually do will be left to future studies.
For other contrasts vs. baseline which are not of principal
interest to this study we report activations that survive a peak
voxel threshold of P
FWE
< 0.05, with familywise error correction
over the whole brain volume.
Co-ordinates in millimeters are given in Montreal
Neurological Institute (MNI) space (Evans et al., 1993).
RESULTS
BEHAVIORAL DATA
Beauty ratings
The formula most consistently rated as beautiful (average rat-
ing of 0.8667), both before and during the scans, was Leonhard
Euler’s identity
1 + e
iπ
= 0
which links 5 fundamental mathematical constants with three
basic arithmetic operations, each occurring once; the one most
consistently rated as ugly (average rating of 0.7333) was
Srinivasa Ramanujans infinite s eries for 1/π,
1
π
=
2
2
9801
k =0
(
4k
)
!
(
1103 + 26390k
)
(
k!
)
4
396
4k
which expresses the reciprocal of π as an infinite sum.
Other highly rated equations included the Pythagorean iden-
tity, the identity between exponential and trigonometric func-
tions derivable from Euler’s formula for complex analysis, and the
Cauchy-Riemann equations (Data Sheet 1: EquationsForm.pdf—
Equations2,5,and54).Formulaecommonlyratedasneutral
included Euler’s formula for polyhedral triangulation, the Gauss
Bonnet theorem and a formulation of the Spectral theorem (Data
Sheet 1: EquationsForm.pdf—Equations 3, 4, and 52). Low rated
equations included Riemanns functional equation, the smallest
number expressible as the sum of two cubes in two different
ways,andanexampleofanexactsequencewheretheimage
of one morphism equals the kernel of the next (Data Sheet 1:
EquationsForm.pdf—Equations 15, 45, and 59).
Pre-, post-, and scan-time ratings
In pre-scan beauty ratings, each subject rated each of the 60 equa-
tions according to beauty on a scale of 5(Ugly)through0
(Neutral) to +5 (Beautiful) while during scan time ratings, sub-
jects rated each equation into the three categories of Ugly, Neutral
or Beautiful.
Post-scan understanding ratings. After scanning, subjects rated
each equation according to their comprehension of the equa-
tion, from 0 (no comprehension whatsoever) to 3 (Profound
understanding).
An excel file containing raw behavioral data is provided as Data
Sheet 3: B ehavioralData.xlsx., which gives the following eight
tables:
Table 1: Pre-scan beauty ratings for each equation by subject
Table 2: Scan-time equation numbers by subject, session
and trial
Table 3: Scan-time beauty ratings for each equation by subject
Table 4: Scan-time beauty rating s by subject, session, and trial
Table 5: Scan-time beauty ratings by subject—Session and
experiment totals
Table 6: Post-scan understanding ratings by subject
Table 7: Post-scan understanding ratings by subject, session,
and trial
Table 8: Post-scan understanding ratings by subject—Session
and experiment totals.
The pre-scan beauty ratings were used to assemble the equa-
tions into three groups, one containing 20 low-rated, another
20 medium-rated, and a third 20 high-rated equations, indi-
vidually for each subject. These three allocations were used to
organize the sequence of equations viewed during each of the
four scanning sessions so that each session contained 5 low-
rated, 5 medium-rated, and 5 high-rated equations. Each subject
then re-rated the equations during the scan as Ugly, Neutral,
or Beautiful. In an ideal case, each subject would identify 5
Ugly, 5 Neutral, and 5 Beautiful equations in each session. In
fact, this did not happen. Figure 2A shows the frequency dis-
tribution of pre-scan beauty ratings for all 15 subjects; it is
positively skewed, indicating that more equations were rated as
beautiful than ugly. This is reflected in the frequency distr ibu-
tion of the scan-time beauty ratings (Figure 2B)which,again,
shows a bias for beautiful equations. Figure 2C shows the rela-
tionship between pre-scan and scan-time beauty ratings. There
was a highly significant positive correlation (Pearsons r = 0.612
for 898 values, p < 0.001) but there were departures; for exam-
ple, one equation received a pre-scan rating of 4butwas
classed as Beautiful at scan-time and three equations with a
pre-scan rating of 5 were subsequently classed as Ugly. These
infrequent departures are not of great concern providing there
was still a reasonable ratio of Ugly: Neutral: Beautiful scan-time
designations for each session, which was the case. Ideally this
ratio would always be 5:5:5 but, due to the predominance of
Beautiful over Ugly scan-time ratings, we twice recorded 0:7:8
and 1:5:9 for particular sessions (see Table 5 in Data Sheet 3:
BehavioralData.xlsx). Other sessions in general showed more
equable ratios and, even with an extreme ratio such as 0:7:8, a
relationship between Neutral and B eautiful equations could still
be established.
The frequency distribution of post-scan understanding ratings
is given in Figure 2D, which shows that more of the equations
were well understood, as would be expected from a group of
expert mathematicians. Figure 2E shows that there was a highly
significant positive correlation (Pearsons r = 0.413 for 898 val-
ues, p < 0.001) between understanding and scan-time beauty
ratings. In this case, departures from a fully correlated relation-
ship allow us to separate out effects of beauty from those of
understanding, so that, for example, i n the well understood cate-
gor y (3) the ratio of Ugly: Neutral: Beautiful is 31:97:205. In order
to analyze scanning data with regard to understanding ratings we
would ideally have equal ratios of the four understanding ratios
(0, 1, 2, and 3) in each scanning session. These ratios are recorded
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 68
| 4
Zeki et al. Neural correlates of mathematical beauty
FIGURE 2 | Summary of Behavioral data. Behavioral data scores
summated over all 15 subjects. (A) Frequency distribution of pre-scan
beauty ratings. (B) Frequency distribution of scan-time beauty ratings.
(C) Pre-scan beauty ratings plotted against scan-time beauty ratings.
(D) Frequency distribution of post-scan understanding ratings.
(E) Post-scan understanding ratings plotted against scan-time beauty
ratings. Numbers in brackets give the count for each group. Area of each
circle is proportional to the count for that group.
in Table 8 in Data Sheet 3: BehavioralData.xlsx. We occasionally
find missing categories in some sessions (such as 0:2:4:9) but we
could still establish a relationship w hen one category is missing in
a particular session.
Post-scan questionnaires regarding subjective (emotional)
experiences
Mathematical subjects were as well given four questions to
answer, post-scan. One subject did not respond to this part of the
questionnaire, leaving us with 14 subjects. To the question: When
you consider a particularly beautiful equation, do you experience
an emotional response?,”9gaveanunqualied“Yes,”1reported
a “shiver of appreciation, 1 reported being “ a bit excited, 1
reported The same kind of response as when hearing a beauti-
ful piece of music, or seeing a particularly appealing painting, 1
reported that “the feeling is visceral” and 1 was “unsure To the
question: Do you derive pleasure, happiness or satisfact ion from
a beautiful equation?” 14 subjects answered affirmatively; all 14
also gave a positive response to the question: Is there any math-
ematical equation which, in the past, you have found particularly
beautiful and, if so, was it among the list of equations which we
gave you?” but some regretted that variations of the equations
were not on the list [e.g., the Einstein field equations, related to
equation 60 (contracted Bianchi identity), and Cauchy’s integral
formula for the special case where n = 1 (Equation 29)]; three
regretted that the following equations were not on the list: the
analytical solution of the Abel integral equation, Noether’s the-
orem, the Euler- Lagrange and the Liouville, Navier-Stokes and
Hamilton equations, Newtons Second Law (F = ma), and the
relativistic Dirac equation. Finally, variable answers were given to
the question:“Do you experience a heightened state of conscious-
ness when you contemplate a beautiful equation?” In summary,
our subjects had an emotional experience when viewing equa-
tions which they had rated as beautiful (the “aesthetic emotion”),
and which they also qualified as satisfying or pleasurable. They
also showed a very sophisticated knowledge of mathematics, by
specifying equations that they considered particularly beautiful
(which the y had known), as well as by the regret expressed at
not finding, in our list, equations that they consider especially
beautiful.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 68
| 5
Zeki et al. Neural correlates of mathematical beauty
Non-mathematical subjects
We also tried to gauge the reaction of 12 non-mathematical
subjects to viewing the same equations. This was, generally,
an unsatisfactory exercise because many had had some, usu-
ally elementary, mathematical experience [up to GCSE (General
Certificate of Secondary Education) level, commonly taken at
ages 14–16]. Reflecting this, the majority indicated that they
had no understanding of what the equations signified, rating
them 0, although some gave positive beauty ratings to a minor-
ity of the equations. Overall, of the 720 equations distributed
over 12 non-mathematical subjects, 645 (89.6%) were given a 0
rating (no understanding), 49 (6.8%) were given a rating of 1
(vague understanding) and the remainder were rated as 2 (good
understanding) or 3 (profound comprehension). To the question
When you consider a particularly beautiful equation, do you expe-
rience an emotional response?, the majority (9 out of 12) gave a
negative response. Given this, we hypothesized that, when such
non-mathematical subjects gave a positive beauty rating to the
equations, they were doing so on a formal basis, that is to say
on how attractive the form of the equations was to them. This
hypothesis receives support from the contrast to reveal the para-
metric relationship between brain activity and understanding in
mathematicians (see Results).
BRAIN ACTIVATIONS
Parametrically related activity in mOFC with beauty ratings,
independently of understanding
Results should that activity that was parametrically related to the
declared intensity of the experience of mathematical beauty was
confined to field A1 of mOFC (Ishizu and Zeki, 2011), where
there was a significant difference in the BOLD signal when view-
ing equations rated as beautiful on the one hand and as neutral
and ugly on the other (Figure 3). Even though our subjects were
experts, with an understanding of the truths that the equations
depict, we had nevertheless asked them to rate, post-scan, how
well they had understood the formulae, on a scale of 0 (no
comprehension) to 3 (profound understanding) (Data Sheet 2:
UnderstandingForm.pdf), as a way of dissociating understanding
from the experience of beauty. There was a good but imperfect
correlation between their understanding and the beauty ratings
given, in that some formulae that had been understood were
not rated as beautiful (Pearson’s correlation coefficient ranging
from 0.0362 to 0.6826). As described above, separate paramet-
ric modulators were used for understanding and beauty ratings in
the SPM analysis. This allowed us to model both understanding
and beauty effects and examine responses to one that could not
be explained by the other, thus separating out the two faculties
in neural terms. Hence, cr ucially, the parametri cally related activ-
ity in mOFC (Figure 3) was specifically driven by beauty ratings,
after accounting for the effects of understanding.
The mOFC was the only brain region that showed a BOLD
signal that was parametrically related to beauty ratings, sig-
nificant at cluster level (see Tabl e 1A and Figure 3). Previous
studies have nevertheless shown a number of areas that are active
when subjects undertake mathematical tasks (see Arsalidou and
Taylor, 2011 for a meta-analysis) and we observed some activ-
ity (which did not reach significance) in three regions which
previous studies of mathematical cognition had reported to be
active (see Ta ble 1 B). One of these is located in the left angu-
largyrus,oneinthemiddletemporalgyrusandoneinthe
FIGURE 3 | Parametric Activations” with Beauty. (A) Second level
parametric analysis derived from 15 subjects, to show parametric
modulation by scan-time beauty rating (after orthogonalization to
understanding rating). One-sample t-test (df = 14) thresholded at
P
unc
< 0.001 with an extent threshold of 10 voxels, revealed a cluster of
95 voxels in medial orbito-frontal cortex (mOFC) with hot-spots at (6,
56, 2) and (0, 35, 14), significant at cluster level, with familywise error
correction over the whole brain volume. The location and extent of the
cluster is indicated by sections along the three principal axes through the
two hot-spots, pinpointed with blue crosshairs and superimposed on an
anatomical image which was averaged over all 15 subjects. (Note:
“activation in this case relates to a positive parametric relationship; i.e.,
an increase in activity with an increase in scan-time beauty rating. Overall,
as can be seen in (B), these locations were deactivated relative to
baseline, i.e., there was a relative activation in a deactivated region. (B) A
separate categorical analysis, based on scan-time beauty ratings alone,
was used to generate contrast estimates for the three categories Ugly,
Neutral, and Beautiful vs. Baseline at each of the locations in (A).
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 68
| 6
Zeki et al. Neural correlates of mathematical beauty
Table 1 | Activation for the contrast parametric beauty.
Location xyzk
E
P
Clust-FEW
T
14
P
FWE
(A) ACTIVATIONS—PARAMETRIC BEAUTY —SIGNIFICANT AT
CLUSTER LEVEL
mOFC 6562 95 0.047 5.14 0.619
mOFC 0 35 14 4.90 0.733
(B) ACTIVATIONS—PARAMETRIC BEAUTY —NOT REACHING
CLUSTER LEVEL SIGNIFICANCE
R caudate 12 8 19 17 0.703 5.72 0.369
L angular gyrus 36 55 22 28 0.489 4.96 0.705
L middle temporal
gyrus
63 4 17 16 0.725 4.48 0.895
SPM 2nd level analysis (15 subjects). One sample t-test (df = 14). (A) Cluster
level significant activation with familywise error correction over the whole brain
volume (P
Clust-FWE
< 0.05, background threshold P
unc.
< 0.001, extent thresh-
old 10 voxels). Co-ordinates of the two hotspots in the cluster are given in MNI
space. (B) In addition to the significant cluster activation shown in Figure 3 and
tabulated above in (A) there were some clusters which failed to reach signifi-
cance but which are listed here as possible contributors to the main cluster of
activation. For completeness we also give the peak significance at each location,
P
FWE
, with familywise error correction over the whole brain volume. The peak
activation in each cluster is shown in bold with up to two other peaks in the
same cluster listed below.
caudate nucleus. Although they did not attain significance, we
nevertheless document them here and leave it to future studies
to ascertain their possible role in the experience of mathematical
beauty.
Beauty—categorical analyses
A categorical analysis of Beauty ratings vs. Baseline is less sophis-
ticated than a parametric analysis in two respects, for reasons
given in the methods section. Nevertheless, we thought it useful
to employ such an analysis to examine the parameter estimates for
Ugly, Neutral and Beautiful vs. Baseline at the locations in mOFC
identified as significant in the parametric study. Figure 3B shows
the parameter estimates for the three beauty categories vs. base-
line at the two locations in mOFC identified in the parametric
analysis: at (6, 56, 2) and (0, 35, 14). It is evident that, at
both, overall activit y within this area of deactivation was greater
for Beautiful than for Neutral or Ugly stimuli. In neither case is a
linear relationship particularly evident, probably due to the infe-
rior sensitivity of the categorical analysis, for the reasons given
above.
Tab le 2 tabulates the categorical contrast Beauty > Neutral.
There are cluster-level significant activations in mOFC, the left
angular gyrus and the left superior temporal sulcus. The relative
activation within mOFC occurs within a region of de-activation
relative to baseline (see section Cortical de-activations when
viewing mathematical formulae). As with the previous study
of Kawabata and Zeki (2004), parameter estimates show that
it is a change in relative activity within a de-activated mOFC
that correlates with the experience of mathematical beauty. Data
Sheet 4: BeauNeutUglyBase.pdf tabulates activations and deacti-
vations of the Beautiful, Neutral, and Ugly categories relative to
baseline.
Table 2 | Activations for the categorical contrast beautiful > neutral.
Location xyz k
E
P
Clust-FWE
T
14
P
FWE
ACTIVATIONS—BEAUTIFUL > NEUTRAL
L angular gyrus 48 67 22 143 0.015 6.32 0.180
mOFC 12 53 4 285 <0.001 5.52 0.416
mOFC 9505 5.16 0.576
mOFC 9385 4.70 0.790
L superior temporal gyrus 12 29 61 122 0.026 4.98 0.633
L superior temporal gyrus 3 17 67 4.65 0.809
L superior temporal gyrus 18 38 46 4.52 0.858
SPM 2nd level analysis (15 subjects). One sample t-test (df = 14). Cluster level
significant activations corrected for familywise error over the whole brain volume
(P
Clust-FWE
< 0.05, background threshold P
unc.
< 0.001, extent threshold 10 vox-
els). Co-ordinates of hotspots within each cluster are given in MNI space. For
completeness we also give the peak significance at each location, P
FWE
,with
familywise error correction over the whole brain volume. The peak activation in
each cluster is shown in bold with up to two other peaks in the same cluster
listed below.
Activity unrelated to beauty ratings
While the experience of mathematical beauty correlated para-
metrically with activity in mOFC, the contrast All equations >
Baseline showed that many sites were generally active when
subjects viewed the equations (Tabl e 3). These included sites
implicated in a variety of relatively simple arithmetic calcula-
tions and problem solving (Dehaene et al., 1999; Fias et al., 2003;
Anderson et al., 2011; Arsalidou and Taylor, 2011; Wintermute
et al., 2012 for a meta-analysis), and symbol processing (Price
and Ansar i, 2011) as well as activity in three sites in the cere-
bellum, generally ignored in past studies of the mathematical
brain: one of these cerebellar sites, located in Crus I, may be
involved in working memory (Stoodley, 2012), another one,
also located in Crus I, in grouping according to numbers (Zeki
and Stutters, 2013) and the processing of abstract information
(Balsters and Ramnani, 2008) while the third one, located in
the para-flocculus, is involved in smooth pursuit eye move-
ments (Ilg and Their, 2008). That these areas should have been
active when subjects view more complex formulae suggests that
they are also recruited in tasks that go beyond relatively simple
arithmetic calculations and involve more complex mathematical
formulations.
Cortical de-activations when viewing mathematical formulae
As well, the contrast All equations < Baseline revealed widespread
cortical de-activations (Ta bl e 3 ), many in areas not specifically
related to mathematical tasks or aesthetic ratings; their distri-
bution corresponds closely to areas active in the resting state
and de-activated during complex cognitive tasks (Shulman et al.,
1997; Binder et al., 1999), including arithmetic ones (Feng et al.,
2007). The most interesting of these is in mOFC. A Conjunction-
Null analysis (Nichols et al., 2005) of the contrasts “Paramet ric
beauty rating” and “De-activations with Equations, both thresh-
olded at P
unc
< 0.001, showed that this de-activation overlaps the
mOFC activation which correlates parametrically with the expe-
rience of mathematical beauty (Figure 4); unlike other areas of
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 68
| 7
Zeki et al. Neural correlates of mathematical beauty
Table 3 | Activations and de-activations for the contrast all equations
vs. baseline.
Location xyzk
E
T
14
P
FWE
ACTIVATIONS—ALL EQUATIONS > BASELINE
L fusiform gyrus 33 82 8 188 15.20 <0.001
L fusiform gyrus 30 91 514.96 <0.001
R fusiform gyrus 42 79 11 377 14.98 <0.001
R cerebellum Crus I 48 70 29 13.02 <0.001
R fusiform gyrus 39 58 11 10.58 0.001
L inferior temporal gyrus 48 50 4 77 13.40 <0.001
L inferior temporal gyrus 48 61 11 79 11.80 <0.001
L intraparietal sulcus 30 67 46 88 10.29 0.002
L lingual/fusiform gyrus 30 49 49 9.53 0.005
R cerebellum lobule VIII/crus I 6 79 29 45 10.10 0.002
R cerebellum dorsal paraflocculus 30 70 50 13 10.04 0.003
L inferior frontal gyrus 48 14 25 24 9.35 0.006
R intraparietal sulcus 30 58 43 19 8.80 0.013
DE-ACTIVATIONS—ALL EQUATIONS < BASELINE
L precuneus 12 46 43 1188 18.22 <0.001
R precuneus 15 40 43 16.63 <0.001
L precuneus 15 40 52 14.73 <0.001
R fusifom gyrus 60 52 16 179 15.39 <0.001
Fusiform gyrus 63 34 16 10.87 <0.001
Fusiform gyrus 63 22 16 8.25 0.027
R medial orbito-frontal cortex 3325 459 14.91 <0.001
L medial orbito-frontal cortex 623814.25 <0.001
R medial orbito-frontal cortex 12 53 213.37 <0.0 01
R superior temporal gyrus 57 16 5 256 14.53 <0.001
R superior temporal gyrus 63 2 23 14.39 <0.001
R superior temporal gyrus 45 13 811.05 <0.001
L lingual gyrus 12 70 22111.81<0.001
L middle temporal gyrus 60 4 17 44 11.75 <0.001
L superior medial gyrus
(anterior paracingulate)
0 53 22 33 10.76 0.001
L middle frontal gyrus 24 26 40 16 10.46 0.002
R superior frontal gyrus
(anterior paracingulate)
18 50 19 12 10.38 0.002
L lingual gyrus 24 46 2 11 9.41 0.006
R cuneus 12 88 16 11 8.98 0.010
SPM 2nd level analysis (15 subjects). One sample t-test (df = 14). All loci,
in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, are significant, thresholded at
P
FWE
< 0.05 with familywise error correction over the whole brain volume and
with an extent threshold of 10 voxels. The peak activation in each cluster is
shown in bold with the cluster size in voxels (k
E
) with up to two other peaks in
the same cluster listed below.
de-activation, the mOFC de-activation has been posited to be
related to task-unrelated conceptual processing (Shulman et al.,
1997). The overlap between the activation and de-activation sug-
gests that there may be separate compartments or sub-systems
within field A1 of mOFC whose activities correlate with general
cognitive tasks on the one hand and the more specific experience
of beauty on the other.
Parametric “de-activations” with understanding ratings
independent of beauty
As described in the methods, we undertook a second parametric
analysis, with Beauty rating and Understanding rating as first and
second parametric modulators, respectively, to isolate activations
due to understanding alone. The result, shown in Figure 5,is
that a large extent of the occipital visual cortex, comprising many
of its subdivisions, was less active for well-understood equations
(or, put another way, more active for less understood equations).
The significance of this is discussed below (under Beauty and
Understanding).
DISCUSSION
Art and mathematics are, to most, at polar opposites; the former
has a more sensible” source and is accessible to many while the
latter has a high cognitive, intellectual, source and is accessible to
few. Yet both can provoke the aesthetic emotion and arouse an
experience of beauty, although neither all great ar t nor all great
mathematical formulations do so. The experience of mathemati-
cal beauty, considered by Plato (1961a,b) to constitute the highest
form of beauty, since it is derived from the intellect alone and
is concerned with eternal and immutable truths, is also one of
the most abstract emotional experiences. In spite of its abstract
nature, there was, for Clive B ell (1914), a strong relation between
mathematical and artistic beauty because the mathematician feels
an emotion for his speculations which “springs... from the heart
of an abstract science. I wonder, sometimes, whether the appre-
ciators of art and of mathematical solutions are not even more
closely allied, while for Bertrand Russell (1919) The true spirit
of delight, the exaltation, the sense of being more than Man,
which is the touchstone of the highest excellence, is to be found
in mathematics as surely as poetry. Given this, we hypothesized
that it would be likely that the experience of beauty derived from
mathematics would correlate with activity in the same par t of the
emotional brain as that derived from other, more sensible and
perceptually based, sources. Although we approached the exper-
iment with diffidence, given the profoundly different sources for
these different experiences, we were not surprised to find, because
of similarities in the experience of beauty provoked by the differ-
ent sources alluded to above, that the experience of mathematical
beauty correlates with activity in the same brain area(s), princi-
pally field A1 of mOFC, that are active during the experience of
visual, musical, and moral beauty. That the activity there is para-
metrically related to the declared intensity of the experience of
beauty, whatever its source, answers affirmatively a critical ques-
tion in the philosophy of aesthetics, namely whether aesthetic
experiences can be quantified (Gordon, 2005).
Mathematical and artistic beauty have been written of in the
same breath by mathematicians and humanists alike, as arous-
ing the “aesthetic emotion. This implies that there is a com-
mon and abstract nature to the experience of beauty derived
from very different sources. Viewed in that light, the activ-
ity in a common area of the emotional brain that correlates
with the experience of beauty derived from different sources
merely mirrors neurobiologically the s ame powerful and emo-
tional experience of beauty that mathematicians and artists alike
have spoken of.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 68
| 8
Zeki et al. Neural correlates of mathematical beauty
FIGURE 4 | Conjunction of activations with beauty rating and
de-activations with equations. Conjunction-Null of two contrasts,
“Parametric beauty rating” and “De-activations with Equations, both
thresholded at P
unc
< 0.001. De-activations are shown in red, overlapping the
area revealed by the parametric rating, shown in yellow. Numerals refer to
MNI co-ordinates.
FIGURE 5 | Parametric “De-activations” with Understanding. (A) Second
level parametric analysis derived from 15 subjects, to show parametric
modulation by understanding rating (after orthogonalization to scan-time
beauty rating). One-sample t-test (df = 14) thresholded at P
unc
< 0.001 with
an extent threshold of 10 voxels, revealed two peak “de-activations”
significant (P
FWE
< 0.05) with familywise error correction over the whole
brain volume, at (39, 52, 8) (T
14
= 9.01, P
FWE
= 0.010) and at (30, 91,
19) (T
14
= 8.05, P
FWE
= 0.032). A third peak at (27, 85, 7) (T
14
= 7.65,
P
FWE
= 0.050) was just above threshold. The location of each peak is
indicated by sections along the three principal axes, pinpointed with blue
crosshairs and superimposed on an anatomical image which was averaged
over all 15 subjects. (Note: A “de-activation in this case relates to a negative
parametric relationship; i.e., activity decreased as understanding rating
increased. Overall, as can be seen in (B), these locations were significantly
active above baseline). (B) A separate categorical analysis based on
understanding ratings alone was used to generate contrast estimates for the
four understanding categories U0, U1, U2, and U3 vs. baseline at each of the
peak locations in (A).
mOFC AND THE EXPERIENCE OF BEAUTY, PREFERENCE, PLEASURE
AND REWARD
The mOFC is active in a variety of conditions, of which experi-
ences relating to pleasure, reward and hedonic states are the most
interesting in our context. The relationship of the experience of
beauty to that of pleasure and reward has been commonly dis-
cussed in the philosophy of aesthetics, without a clear conclusion
(Gordon, 2005). This is perhaps not surprising , because the three
merge into one another, without clear boundaries between them;
neurologically, activity in mOFC correlates with all three expe-
riences thus reflecting, perhaps, the difficulty of separating these
experiences subjectively. The imperfect distinction between the
three is also reflected in the positive post-scan answers given by
the mathematical subjects to the question whether they experi-
enced pleasure, satisfaction or happiness when viewing equations
that they had rated as beautiful. Whether one can ever experi-
ence beauty without at the same time experiencing a sense of
pleasure and/or reward is doubtful. The converse is not true, in
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 68
| 9
Zeki et al. Neural correlates of mathematical beauty
that something can be experienced as being pleasurable, reward-
ing, or preferred without it being also experienced as beautiful
or arousing the aesthetic emotion. Neurobiologically, the issue
resolves itself around the question of whether a ctivity in the same
or different parts of the mOFC correlates with these different
experiences. The mOFC is a relatively large expanse of cortex
with several cytocrachitectonic subdivisions, including BA 10,
11, 12, and 32 (see Kringelbach, 2005 for a review) as well as
BA 24 (which may more properly be considered as part of the
rostral anterior cingulate cortex) and BA 25. Brodmann maps
are useful general anatomical guides but do not delimit func-
tional areas and major functional subidvisions can be found
within single cytoarchitectonic fields such as, for example, BA
18 (see Zeki, 1979). It is possible that, as with lateral orbito-
frontal cortex, future studies may subdivide mOFC into further
subdivisions based on resting-state connectivity or other criteria
(Kahnt et al., 2012). Hence any reference to Brodmann maps is no
more than a rough guide and must be tentative. Our delineation
(Ishizu and Zeki, 2011) of a field within mOFC, field A1, has the
virtue of delimiting a specific cortical zone with relatively precise
co-ordinates and dimensions, whose activity correlates with the
aesthetic emotion and to which other functional parcellations,
both past and future, can be referred with relative precision. Field
A1 of mOFC has MNI co-ordinates of (3, 41, 8) and a diam-
eter of between 15 and 17 mm (see Ishizu and Zeki, 2011). This
falls in the middle of the active region of mOFC in the present
study, and hence must be located within what we defined as field
A1.Infact,ifoneweretotaketheaverageofthetwohotspots
in the mOFC cluster of the parametric beauty contrast of this
study (namely 3, 45, 8), it will be found to be located 4 mm
fromthecenterofthefieldA1at(3, 41, 8). A survey of the
published evidence shows that functional parcellations cannot be
restricted to single cytoarchitectonic subdivisions comprising the
medial orbital wall of the frontal cortex. Not all rewarding and
pleasurable experiences activate field A1 of mOFC, which occu-
pies mainly BA 32 but probably extends to BA 12 inferiorly and
BA 25 anteriorly. While preference for drinks (McClure et al.,
2004), abstract and predictive rewards and kinetic patterns do
(O’Doherty et al., 2001; Gottfried et al., 2003; Zeki and Stutters,
2011), the hedonic experience of food appears to correlate with a
morelateralpartofmOFCthanfieldA1ofmOFC(Kringelbach
et al., 2003) while the experience of erotic pleasure appears to cor-
relate with a region dorsal to it (Sescousse et al., 2010) (see also
Berridge and Kringelbach, 2013) for a review. These are approxi-
mations that do not allow more definitive conclusions at present,
even with meta-analyses such as the ones provided by Peters and
Büchel (2010) or Kühn and Gallinat (2012). The latter help locate
the general brain regions active with specific experiences but are
not presently capable of pinpointing whether the identical regions
are active, which would require far more detailed conjunction
analyses in studies employing several different categories of hedo-
nic experience. This is even true of the experience of visual and
musical beauty. Though a conjunction analysis reveals that there
is a common part of A1 of mOFC that correlates with both expe-
riences (Ishizu and Zeki, 2011), the overlap is not perfect and it is
possible that different subdivisions, or perhaps different group-
ings of cells within the same general subdivision, are recruited
during different experiences (see Figure 1 of Ishizu and Zeki,
2011). The same is true of the present results in relation to pre-
vious activations of mOFC that correlate with the experience of
visual and musical beauty. Very close correspondence in the active
regions for all three experiences does not preclude that there may
yet be different distributions or subdivisions within field A1 of
mOFC. It is also useful to emphasize here, as we have done in
the past, that the localization of activity in mOFC that correlates
with the experience of mathematical beauty does not imply that
this area alone is responsible for, or underlies, the experience.
As we have shown here and elsewhere, the viewing of mathe-
matical equations also results in activity in cortical areas that are
distinct from the areas engaged w hen viewing paintings which, in
turn, differ from those active when listening to musical excerpts.
ThecommonfactorisactivityinmOFCwhenbeautyinthese
domains is experienced, but mOFC cannot act alone; rather it is
active in concert with other areas which, we suppose, collectively
correlate with the experience of beauty derived from different
sources.
BEAUTY AND UNDERSTANDING
Perhaps one of the most awkward, and at the same time chal-
lenging, aspects of this work was trying to separate out beauty
and understanding. Because the correlation between the two,
though significant, was also imperfect, we were able to do so for
mathematicians. T his of course leaves the question of whether
non-mathematicians, with no understanding whatsoever of the
equations, would also find the equations beautiful. Ideally, one
would want to have subjects who are mathematically totally illiter-
ate, a search that proved difficult. We relied, instead, on a different
approach. Most of our non-mathematician” subjects had a very
imperfect understanding of the equations, even though they had
rated some of them as beautiful; we supposed that they did so on
the basis of the formal qualities of the equations—the forms dis-
played, their symmetrical distribution, etc. We surmised that we
could demonstrate this indirectly, by showing that less well under-
stood equations in our mathematical subjects will, when viewed,
lead to more intense activity in the visual areas. This is what we
found (see Figure 5), and it implies that some combinations of
form are more aesthetically pleasing than others, even if they are
not understood” cognitively (see also quote from Dirac, given
below). What these formal qualities may be requires a s eparate
and detailed study of the beauty of forms, well beyond the scope
of this study. But a parallel may be found in our earlier study,
which shows that there are some configurations of kinetic stimuli
which activate the motion areas of the visual brain more intensely
(Zeki and Stutters, 2011).
IMPLICATIONS FOR FUTURE WORK
The experience of beauty derived from mathematical formula-
tions represents the most extreme case of the experience of beauty
that is dependent on learning and culture. The fact that the expe-
rience of mathematical beauty, like the experience of musical and
visualbeauty,correlateswithactivityinA1ofmOFCsuggests
that there is, neurobiologically, an abstract quality to beauty that
is independent of c ulture and learning. But that there was an
imperfect correlation between understanding and the experience
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 68
| 10
Zeki et al. Neural correlates of mathematical beauty
of beauty and that activity in the mOFC cannot be accounted for
by understanding but by the experience of beauty alone, raises
issues of profound interest for the future. It leads to the capital
question of whether beauty, even in so abstract an area as math-
ematics, is a pointer to what is true in nature, both within our
natureandintheworldinwhichwehaveevolved.Paul Dirac
(1939) put it like this: “There is no logical reason why the (method
of mathematical reasoning should make progress in the study of
natural phenomena) but one has found in practice that it does
work and meets with reasonable success. This must be ascribed
to some mathematical quality in Nature, a qualit y which the
casual observer of Nature would not suspect, but which never-
thelessplaysanimportantroleinNaturesscheme...Whatmakes
the theory of relativity so acceptable to physicists in spite of its
going against the principle of simplicity is its great mathematical
beauty. This is a quality which cannot be defined, any more than
beauty in art can be defined, but which people who study math-
ematics usually have no difficulty in appreciating. The theory of
relativity introduced mathematical beauty to an unprecedented
extent into the description of Nature...We now see that we have
to change the principle of simplicity into a principle of math-
ematical beauty. The research worker, in his efforts to express
the fundamental laws of Nature in mathematical form, should
strive mainly for mathematical beauty. He should still take sim-
plicity into consideration in a subordinate way to beauty. It often
happens that the requirements of simplicity and of beauty are
the same, but where the y clash the latter must take precedence
(ellipses added). In similar vein, Hermann Weyl is recorded as
having said, “My work always tried to unite the true with the
beautiful; but when I had to choose one or the other, I usually
chose the beautiful” (Dyson, 1956). Relevant here is the story
of Weyl’s mathematical formulations, which tried to reconcile
electromagnetism with relativity. Rejected at first (by Einstein)
because it was thought to conflict with experimental evidence,
it came subsequently to be accepted but only after the advent of
quantum mechanics, which led to a new interpretation of Weyl’s
equations. Hence the perceived beauty of his mathematical for-
mulations ultimately predicted truths even before the full facts
were known.
If the experience of mathematical beauty is not strictly related
to understanding (of the equations), what can the source of math-
ematical beauty be? That is perhaps more difficult to account for
in mathematics than in visual art or music. Whereas the source
for the latter can be accounted for, at least theoretically, by pre-
ferred harmonies in nature or preferred distribution of forms or
colors (see Bell, 1914; Zeki and Stutters, 2011; Zeki, 2013), it is
more difficult to make such a correspondence in mathematics.
The Platonic t radition would emphasize that mathematical for-
mulations are experienced as beautiful because they give insights
into the fundamental structure of the universe (see Breitenbach,
2013). For Immanuel Kant, by contrast, the aesthetic experience
is as well grounded in our own nature because, for him, Aesthetic
judgments may thus be regarded as expressions of our feeling that
something makes sense to us” (Breitenbach, 2013). We believe
that what “makes sense” to us is grounded in the workings of
our brain, which has evolved within our physical environment.
Dirac (1939) wrote: “the mathematician plays a game in which
he himself invents the rules while the physicist plays a game in
which the rules are provided by Nature, but as time goes on
it becomes increasingly evident that the rules which the math-
ematician finds interesting are the same as those which Nature
has chosen and therefore that in the choice of new branches of
mathematics, “One should be influenced very much... by con-
siderations of mathematical beauty” (ellipsis added). Hence the
work we report here, as well as our previous work, highlights
further the extent to which even future mathematical formula-
tions may, by being based on beauty, reveal something about
our brain on the one hand, and about the extent to which our
brain organization reveals something about our universe on the
other.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust London,
Strategic Award “Neuroesthetics” 083149/Z/07/Z. We are ver y
grateful to Karl Friston for his many good suggestions during the
course of this work.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found
online at: http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fnhum.
2014.00068/abstract
We include four datasheets containing experimental details
which are not essential for general understanding of the article:
Data Sheet 1: EquationsForm.pdf—The equation beauty-rating
questionnaire.
Data Sheet 2: UnderstandingForm.pdf—The equation under-
standing questionnaire.
Data Sheet 3: BehavioralData.xlsx—Tables of behavioral data.
Data Sheet 4: BeauNeutUglyBase.pdf—Tables of categorical
beauty activations vs. baseline.
REFERENCES
Anderson, J. R., Betts, S., Ferris, J. L., and Fincham, J. (2011). Cognitive and
metacognitive activity in mathematical problem solving: prefrontal and parietal
patterns. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 11, 52–67. doi: 10.3758/s13415-010-
0011-0
Arsalidou, M., and Taylor, M. J. (2011). Is 2+2=4? Metanalysis of brain
areas needed for numbers and calculation. Neuroimage 54, 2382–2393. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.009
Atiyah, M. (1973). Mathematics: art and science. Bull. Amer. Math. Soc. 43, 87–88.
doi: 10.1090/S0273-0979-05-01095-5
Atiyah, M. (2002). Hermann Weyl: 1885-1955, biographical memoirs. Natl. Acad.
Sci. 82, 1–17.
Balsters, J. H., and Ramnani, N. (2008). Symbolic representations of action in the
human cerebellum. Neuroimage 43, 388–398. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.
07.010
Bell, C. (1914). Art. London: Chatto and Windus.
Berridge, K. C., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2013). Neuroscience of affect: brain mech-
anisms of pleasure and displeasure. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 294–303. doi:
10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.017
Binder,J.R.,Frost,J.A.,Hammeke,T.A.,Bellgowan,R.S.F.,Rao,S.M.,and
Cox, R. W. (1999). Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state:
a functional MRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 80–93. doi: 10.1162/0898929995
63265
Blood, A. J., Zatorre, R. J., Bermudez, P., and Evans, A. C. (1999). Emotional
responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic
brain regions. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 382–387. doi: 10.1038/7299
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 68
| 11
Zeki et al. Neural correlates of mathematical beauty
Breitenbach, A. (2013). Beauty in proofs: kant on aesthetics in mathematics. Eur. J.
Philos. 21, 1–23. doi: 10.1111/ejop.12021
Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., and Tsivki, S. (1999). Sources of
mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science 284,
970–974. doi: 10.1126/science.284.5416.970
Dirac, P. (1939). The relation between mathematics and physics. Proc. R. Soc. Edin.
59, 122–129.
Dyson, F. (1956). Obituary of Hermann Weyl. Nature 177, 457–458.
Evans, A. C., Collins, D. L., Mills, S. R., Brown, E. D., Kelly, R. L., and Peters,
T. M. (1993). 3D statistical neuroanatomical models from 305 MRI vol-
umes, in Nuclear Science Symposium and Medical Imaging Conference, 1993.,
1993 IEEE Conference Record, Vol. 3 (San Francisco, CA), 1813–1817. doi:
10.1109/NSSMIC.1993.373602
Feng, H., Zhang, Y., Tang, Y., Jin, J., Dong, F., Feng, S., et al. (2007). Deactivations
during the numerical processing. Chinese Sci. Bull. 52, 1807–1812. doi:
10.1007/s11434-007-0256-3
Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynovet, B., Dupony, P., and Orban, G. A. (2003). Parietal
representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15,
47–56. doi: 10.1162/089892903321107819
Friston,K.,Asburner,T.,Kiebel,S.J.,Nichols,T.E.,andPenny,W.D.(eds.).
(2006). Statistical Parametric Mapping: The Analysis of Functional Brain Images.
Amsterdam and London: Academic Press.
Friston, K. J., Josephs, O., Zar ahn, E., Holmes, A. P., Rouquette, S., and Poline, J.-B.
(2000). To smooth or not to smooth? Bias and efficiency in fMRI time-series
analysis. Neuroimage 12, 196–208. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.060
Friston,K.J.,Worsley,K.J.,Frackowiak,R.S.J.,Mazziotta,J.C.,andEvans,A.C.
(1994). Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent.
Hum. Brain Mapp. 1, 210–220. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460010306
Gordon, G. (2005). Philosophy of The Arts. 3rd Edn. Oxford: Routledge.
Gottfried, J. A., O’Doherty, J., and Dolan, R. J. (2003). Encoding predictive reward
value in human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Science 301, 1104–1107. doi:
10.1126/science.1087919 9.10
Ilg, U. J., and Their, P. (2008). The neural basis of smooth pursuit eye
movements in the rhesus monkey brain. Brain Cogn. 68, 229–240. doi:
10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.014
Ishizu, T., and Zeki, S. (2011). Toward a brain-based theory of beauty. PLoS ONE
6:e21852. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021852
Kahnt, T., Chang, L. J., Park, S. Q., Heinzle, J., and Haynes, J.-D. (2012).
Connectivity-based parcellation of the human orbitofrontal cortex. J. Neurosci.
32, 6240–6250. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0257-12.2012
Kawabata, H., and Zeki, S. (2004). Neural correlates of beauty. J. Neurophysiol. 91,
1699–2004. doi: 10.1152/jn.00696.2003
Kringelbach, M. H., O’Doher ty, J., Rolls, E. T., and Andrews, C. (2003). Activation
of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated
with its subjective pleasantness. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1064–1071. doi: 10.1093/cer-
cor/13.10.1064
Kringelbach, M. L. (2005). The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to
hedonic experience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 691–702. doi: 10.1038/nrn1747
Kühn, S., and Gallinat, J. (2012). The neural correlates of subjective pleasantness.
Neuroimage 61, 289–294. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.065
McClure, S. M., Li, J., Tomlin, D., Cypert, K. S., Montague, L. M., and Montague,
P. R. (2004). Neural correlates of behavioral preference for culturally familiar
drinks. Neuron 44, 379–387. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.019
Nichols, T., Brett, M., Andersson, J., Wager, T., and Poline, J.-B. (2005). Valid con-
junction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage 25, 653–660. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.005
O’Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., and Andrews, C. (2001).
Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal
cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 95–102. doi: 10.1038/82959
Peters, J., and Büchel, C. (2010). Neural representations of subjective reward value.
Behav. Brain Res. 213, 135–141. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.031
Plato. (1929). Timaeus and Critias, 30b AE Taylor (trans). London: Methuen.
Plato. (1961a). Timaeus, (54a), from: Plato: The Collected Dialogues, Bollingen Series
LXXI, Trans eds E. Hamilton and H. Cairns. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
Press.
Plato. (1961b). Timaeus, (29a,b), from: Plato: The Collected Dialogues, Bollingen
Series LXXI, Trans eds E. Hamilton and H. Cairns. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Price, G. R., and Ansari, D. (2011). Symbol processing in the left angular gyrus:
evidence from passive perception of digits. Neuroimage 57, 1205–1211. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.035
Russell, B. (1919). Mysticism and Logic and Other Essays. London: Allen and
Unwin.
Sescousse, G., Redouté, J., and Dreher, J.-C. (2010). The architecture of reward
value coding in the human orbitofrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 30, 13095–13104.
doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3501-10.2010
Shulman, G. L., Fiez, J. A., Corbetta, M., Buchner, R. L., Miezin, F. M., Raichle, M.
E., et al. (1997). Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases
in cerebral cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 648–663. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.
9.5.648
Stoodley, C. J. (2012). The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional
imaging studies. Cerebellum 11, 352–365. doi: 10.1007/s12311-011-0260-7
Tsukiura, T., and Cabeza, R. (2011). Shared brain activity for aesthetic and moral
judgments: implications for the Beauty-is-Good stereotype. Soc. Cogn. Affect.
Neurosci. 6, 138–148. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq025
Wintermute, S., Betts, S., Ferris, J. L., Fincham, J. M., and Anderson, J. R. (2012).
Brain networks supporting execution of mathematical skills versus acquisition
of new mathematical competence. PLoS ONE 7:e50154. doi: 10.1371/jour-
nal.pone.0050154
Zeki, S. (2013). Clive bell’s significant form and the neurobiology of aesthetics.
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:730. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00730
Zeki, S., and Romaya, J. P. (2010). The brain reaction to viewing faces of opposite-
and same-sex romantic partners. PLoS ONE 5:15802. doi: 10.1371/jour-
nal.pone.0015802
Zeki, S., and Stutters, J. (2011). A brain-derived metric for preferred kinetic stimuli.
Open Biol. 2, 1–8. doi: 10.1098/rsob.120001
Zeki, S., and Stutters, J. (2013). Functional specialization and generalization for
grouping of stimuli based on colour and motion. Neuroimage 73, 156–166. doi:
10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.001
Zeki, S. M. (1979). Zu Brodmanns area 18 und 19. Exp. Brain Res. 36, 195–197.
doi: 10.1007/BF00238478
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was con-
ducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be
construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Received: 11 November 2013; accepted: 28 January 2014; published online: 13
February 2014.
Citation: Zeki S, Romaya JP, Benincasa DMT and Atiyah MF (2014) The experience
of mathematical beauty and its neural correlates. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:68. doi:
10.3389/fnhum.2014.00068
This article was submitted to the journal Frontiers in Human Neuroscience.
Copyright © 2014 Zeki, Romaya, Benincasa and Atiyah. This is an open-access arti-
cle distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY).
The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the
original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publicat ion in this
journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or
reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2014 | Volume 8 | Article 68
| 12

Discussion

$$1+e^{i\pi}=0$$ Euler's Identity is a special case of Euler's Formula that establishes the fundamental relationship between the trigonometric functions and the complex exponential function. Expert mathematicians had an emotional response described as an "aesthetic emotion" when viewing equations they rated as beautiful. This emotional experience was also characterized as satisfying or pleasurable. The subjects demonstrated a deep understanding and appreciation of mathematics by identifying specific equations they found particularly beautiful, and expressing regret when some of their favorite equations were not included in the list. Art and mathematics are sometimes perceived as polar opposites. They can both provoke aesthetic emotions and arouse an experience of beauty. Despite its abstract nature, there is a strong relation between mathematical and artistic beauty, as both mathematicians and art appreciators feel a similar sense of delight and exaltation. Plato considered mathematical beauty to be the highest form of beauty. > ***"The beauty of mathematical formulations lies in abstracting, in simple equations, truths that have universal validity. [...] the experience of mathematical beauty has much in common with that derived from other sources, even though mathematical beauty has a much deeper intellectual source than visual or musical beauty, which are more “sensible” and perceptually based."*** You can find the Data Sheet with all of the 60 mathematical equations here: [The Beauty of Mathematics - Equations](https://fermatslibrary.com/p/413aae33) The results show a strong positive correlation between the level of understanding and scan-time beauty ratings, indicating that well-understood equations were generally perceived as more beautiful. The relationship was not perfect, allowing the researchers to separate the effects of beauty from those of understanding. Authors note that for ideal analysis they would prefer equal ratios of the four understanding categories in each scanning session. > ***"Perhaps one of the most awkward, and at the same time challenging, aspects of this work was trying to separate out beauty and understanding. Because the correlation between the two, though significant, was also imperfect, we were able to do so for mathematicians. This of course leaves the question of whether non-mathematicians, with no understanding whatsoever of the equations, would also find the equations beautiful."*** ### TL;DR This paper explores the fascinating connection between the experience of beauty in mathematics and art, and how the brain processes these abstract emotional experiences. The authors used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to identify the brain regions activated when mathematicians experience mathematical beauty. The researchers discovered that: - The experience of mathematical beauty correlates with activity in the same brain area as the experience of beauty derived from other sources, such as art or music. - The experience of mathematical beauty is related to activity in brain areas associated with reward, emotion, and cognition, suggesting a complex interplay between these processes. - Mathematical beauty is a combination of the understanding of mathematical concepts and the emotional response they evoke. - There seems to exist a strong link between mathematical and artistic beauty. Understanding the neural basis of mathematical beauty may help educators design teaching methods that foster a deeper appreciation for the subject. > ***"Art and mathematics are, to most, at polar opposites; the former has a more “sensible” source and is accessible to many while the latter has a high cognitive, intellectual, source and is accessible to few."***